The Impact of Cell Phone Use on Young Drivers' Road Safety
Verified
Added on 2023/06/04
|14
|5659
|418
AI Summary
This report analyzes the impact of cell phone use on young drivers' road safety and proposes prevention strategies. It includes research hypotheses, sample population, procedure, materials, and results.
Contribute Materials
Your contribution can guide someone’s learning journey. Share your
documents today.
RUNNING HEAD: PSYC105 REPORT 20181 PSYC105 Report 2018 Name Institution
Secure Best Marks with AI Grader
Need help grading? Try our AI Grader for instant feedback on your assignments.
PSYC105 REPORT 20182 PSYC105 report 2018 Introduction Road traffic nowadays is naturally dangerous. Compared to other modes of transport, for example, air traffic and railways, the road traffic system was not planned with safety as a jumping-off point. Notably, cell phone usage among young drivers has turned out to be increasingly common in the latest years (Cazzulino, Burke, Muller, Arbogast, & Upperman, 2014). Young individuals are the most expected to accept the application of new technology and least expected to recognize the risks related with cell phone use while driving (CPWD; described in this context as talking on the phone only) and sending a text while driving (TextWD) (Chaurand, Bossart, & Delhomme, 2015). Owing to rawness, young drivers are most at risk when using cell phones while driving and thus ought to be the mark of most of the prevention measures (Tay & Watson, 2002). As we are all aware, driving is an intricate process that involves eyes-hand-foot coordination (Darçın & Alkan, 2015 and thus futile coordination usually results in road accidents. It has been established that the young drivers contribute to more accidents because of their highest affinities of using cell phone whiles driving compared to other groups of people. As stated by Waddell and Wiener (2014), activities involving the cell phone, for example, calling and messaging could contribute to the distraction of the driver. Previous studies have also shown that the cell phone use while driving decreases the performance of a driver in many areas (Hallett, Lambert, & Regan, 2012; White, Hyde, Walsh, & Watson, 2010). For instance, drivers spend less time observing vehicle’s mirrors and their instruments when using a cell phone while driving (Waddell & Wiener, 2014). Previous research has disclosed that young adults particularly males have a habit of using a cell phone while driving more than female or older drivers (Darçın & Alkan, 2015; Waddell & Wiener, 2014; White, Hyde, Walsh, & Watson, 2010). The findings are still not conclusive. For that reason, this study aims to identify the contributory factors that affect young drivers to participate in TextWD and CPWD and propose a basis for prevention strategies and campaigns that can effectively thwart recent and future generations from using cell phones while driving.
PSYC105 REPORT 20183 Research Hypothesis The three hypothesis of this present study can be stated as Gain-framed, compared to loss-framed, safety messages about mobile phone use while driving will be more effective in changing people's intentions towards this behaviour ; high issue involvement inducing, compared to low issue involvement inducing, safety messages will be more effective in changing people's intentions about unsafe driving behaviour and; gain-framed safety messages about mobile phone use will be most effective when paired with a high issue involvement inducing message Method Sample Population The survey was rolled online and completed by 892 teens and young adult drivers, but only 343 were considered as the rest were deemed incomplete. All the 343 informants met all of the screening standards: had learner’s permit or a driver’s license, drove more often, possessed a cell phone and were aged between 15 and 26 years. Eighty six (25.07%) of the respondents were male and 255 (74.34) were female. Procedure The survey was piloted using Qualtrics® software (from August 24 to September 2018). The respondents were invited to cooperate on a web survey about the association between the use of mobile phone while driving and the age. The enumerator clarified to the informants what the survey was all about before its start and presented a consent form from the Macquarie University, Department of Psychology. Additionally, the participants were assured that their identity would remain anonymous throughout the study and their responses would only be used for study purpose. Then, all the participants were asked to evaluate the content of a set of questions about the use of the phone while driving and some road safety parameters. On average, it took the participants 15-20 minutes to complete the online questionnaire. Materials The road carnage imaged used in the introduction part of the questionnaire was intentionally used
PSYC105 REPORT 20184 to present the content of the road safety and ramifications when these regulations are violated to sway the attention of the target respondents towards giving honest answers about various parameters of the study topic (Tay, 2005). This image was also accompanied by phone texting image by a person who is driving and very reluctant to observe what is happening on the road (Ruiter, Abraham, & Kok, 2001). At the end of the questionnaire, a thank you picture was used to commend the respondents for sparing their time to getting involved in the survey (Nathanail & Adamos, 2013). The survey collected a measure of drivers’ intentions to take/make phone calls (talking) in addition to their intentions to make/take text messages (texting) while driving. Survey Validation The survey elements were tested for reliability, and the discriminant and convergent validity was also evaluated for each item. Cronbach’s alpha was applied to test the reliability for each of the 15 constructs in the model (Gupta & Rastogi, 2016). Based on this examination, if Cronbach’s alpha for a particular construct was found to be less than 0.71, it was not used from the final analysis (Corben, Logan, Fanciulli, Farley, & Cameron, 2010). Nonetheless, if the minimum criteria was deemed achievable by dropping one or more stuffs while keeping no less than 2 items, only the items that were considered unreliable were omitted, not the whole construct. Results Descriptive Statistics Descriptive statistics analysis (i.e., frequencies, minimum and maximum values, confidence intervals and standard-deviation) were completed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 22.0. Demographics Demographic data of the respondents was captured under age, gender status, driving experience, license ownership and use of mobile phones. As for the age parameter, majority of the participants were aged appeoximately19 years. Variable |ObsMeanStd. Dev.MinMax
Paraphrase This Document
Need a fresh take? Get an instant paraphrase of this document with our AI Paraphraser
PSYC105 REPORT 20185 -------------+--------------------------------------------------------- AGE |34319.2071.6826461726 In addition, this survey saw the respondent(s) with the maximum age (highest age) being aged 26 years while the least/minimum age representation in the survey was 17 years. In terms of gender, the majority of the participants were female (74.34%). Their male counterparts constituted only 25.07% of the total population of respondents whose responses were considered for the analysis (343). Sex |Freq.PercentCum. ------------+----------------------------------- Male |8625.0725.07 Female |25574.3499.42 Other |20.58100.00 ------------+----------------------------------- Total |343100.00 The survey also inquired about the driving experience and behavior which was quantified in terms of years of owning a valid license. From the findings, most of the respondents had owned their licenses for not less than 1 year with 10 years being the most years that some of these respondents had possessed them; otherwise stated, they had 10 years driving experience. Years Licensed Variable |ObsMeanStd. Dev.MinMax -------------+--------------------------------------------------------- YearsLicensed|3423.0184211.731034010 Asked about the type of licence they held, majority of the respondents indicated that they owned PS- driving licence (62.10%) followed by learners licences with a representation of 21%. Still, some of the respondents indicated they did not have any licence. Type of license License type |Freq.PercentCum. ----------------------+----------------------------------- None |61.751.75
PSYC105 REPORT 20186 Learners |7421.5723.32 Ps |21362.1085.42 Full or heavy vehicle |4312.5497.96 Motorcycle |72.04100.00 ----------------------+----------------------------------- Total |343100.00 About the use of mobile phone while driving question, the respondents responded as shown below. Mobile phone use while driving How often do YOU use your | Phone while driving?|Freq.PercentCum. ---------------------------+----------------------------------- Three or more times a week |4713.7013.70 Once or twice a week |9327.1140.82 Once or twice a month |3510.2051.02 Less than once a month |6117.7868.80 Never |10731.20100.00 ---------------------------+----------------------------------- Total |343100.00 From the recorded outcomes regarding this aspect of study, it is evident that they do not use their phones while driving (31%). Still, a significant proportion of the respondents confessed to using their mobile phones while driving once or twice a week (27%) with another group stating that they use mobile phone while driving three or more times a week (13.70%). The survey also wanted to assess the addiction level and thus tested the respondents about the use of mobile phone in other places and not necessarily while only driving. Accordingly, most of the respondents indicated they use their phones three or more times a week (56.56%) and others once or twice a week (34.40%). These two groups formed more than 80% of the 343 respondents that participated in this survey Use of mobile phone in other places How often do you think| OTHERS use their phone?|Freq.PercentCum. ---------------------------+----------------------------------- Three or more times a week |19456.5656.56 Once or twice a week |11834.4090.96 Once or twice a month |216.1297.08 Less than once a month |61.7598.83 Never |41.17100.00
PSYC105 REPORT 20187 ---------------------------+----------------------------------- Total |343100.00 Hypothesis Testing Hypotheses were tested with independent samples t-tests.Below are the results of each hypotheses on three measures that were being tested. Talking • Hypothesis 1: Gain-framed vs Loss-frame conditions The first hypothesis of this study in relation to talking while driving was gain-framed, compared to loss- framed, safety messages about mobile phone use while driving will be more effective in changing people’s intentions towards this behavior. The results of the t-test in relation to this study are presented below. Two-sample t test with equal variances ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Variable |ObsMeanStd. Err.Std. Dev.[95% Conf. Interval] ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- Loss |1696.843195.902980511.738755.0605448.625846 Gain |1745.6752871.00089513.202713.699757.650825 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- combined |3436.250729.674844612.49834.923367.578097 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- diff |1.1679081.35033-1.4881173.823933 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ diff = mean(Loss) - mean(Gain)t =0.8649 Ho: diff = 0degrees of freedom =341 Ha: diff < 0Ha: diff != 0Ha: diff > 0 Pr(T < t) = 0.8062Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.3877Pr(T > t) = 0.1938 Since Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.3877 is less than p-value 0.05 , the null hypothesis was rejected; the alternative was accepted i.e. loss-framed, compared to gained-framed, safety messages about mobile phone use while driving will be more effective in changing people’s intentions towards this behaviour. • Hypothesis 2: High-issue involvement vs Low Issue involvement conditions
Secure Best Marks with AI Grader
Need help grading? Try our AI Grader for instant feedback on your assignments.
PSYC105 REPORT 20188 The second hypothesis of this studyin relation to talking while drivingwas high issue involvement inducing, compared to low issue involvement inducing, safety messages will be more effective in changing people’s intentions about unsafe driving behaviour and was tested as shown below. Two-sample t test with equal variances ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Variable |ObsMeanStd. Err.Std. Dev.[95% Conf. Interval] ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- Low |1824.431319.84598711.412992.7620536.100584 High |1618.3074531.05300213.36116.22787810.38703 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- combined |3436.250729.674844612.49834.923367.578097 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- diff |-3.8761351.337841-6.507594-1.244676 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ diff = mean(Low) - mean(High)t = -2.8973 Ho: diff = 0degrees of freedom =341 Ha: diff < 0Ha: diff != 0Ha: diff > 0 Pr(T < t) = 0.0020Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.0040Pr(T > t) = 0.9980 From the results, Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.0040 which is less than the p-value 0.05 hence the null objective was rejected and the alternative hypothesis accepted in its places; low issue involvement inducing compared to high issue involvement inducing, safety messages will be more effective in changing people’s intentions about unsafe driving behaviour. • Hypothesis 3: Gain-framed High-issue involvement vs. Loss-framed High-issue involvement The third hypothesis of this studyin relation to talking while drivingwas gain-framed safety messages about mobile phone use will be most effective when paired with a high issue involvement inducing message and was subsequently tested as shown below. Two-sample t test with equal variances ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Variable |ObsMeanStd. Err.Std. Dev.[95% Conf. Interval] ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- Loss |818.370371.39909512.591865.58608211.15466 Gain |808.243751.58503714.1775.08881311.39869 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- combined |1618.3074531.05300213.36116.22787810.38703 ---------+--------------------------------------------------------------------
PSYC105 REPORT 20189 diff |.12662042.112633-4.0458224.299063 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ diff = mean(Loss) - mean(Gain)t =0.0599 Ho: diff = 0degrees of freedom =159 Ha: diff < 0Ha: diff != 0Ha: diff > 0 Pr(T < t) = 0.5239Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.9523Pr(T > t) = 0.4761 Since the Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.9523 was more than the p-value, the null hypothesis was not rejected (failed to reject null hypothesis). Thus, gain-framed safety messages about mobile phone use will be most effective when paired with a high issue involvement inducing message. Subsequently, the graph below summarizes the meanchange in intention to talk on the phone while driving between the four groups; summarizes the above hypotheses. Low InvolvementHigh Involvement 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Gain-framedLoss-framed Change in Intention to Talk Graph representing the mean change in intention to talk on the phone while driving between the four groups. From the graph it is evident that loss-framed, compared to gained-framed, safety messages about mobile phone use while driving will be more effective in changing people’s intentions towards this behaviour. Additionally, the graph reveals that low issue involvement inducing compared to high issue involvement inducing, safety messages will be more effective in changing people’s intentions about unsafe driving behaviour. Texting
PSYC105 REPORT 201810 • Hypothesis 1: Gain-framed vs Loss-frame conditions The first hypothesis of this study in relation to texting while driving was gain-framed, compared to loss- framed, safety messages about mobile phone use while driving will be more effective in changing people’s intentions towards this behavior. The results of the t-test in relation to this study are presented below. Two-sample t test with equal variances ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Variable |ObsMeanStd. Err.Std. Dev.[95% Conf. Interval] ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- Loss |1694.9556211.00202213.026282.9774456.933798 Gain |1744.5632181.04610713.79912.4984436.627994 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- combined |3434.75656.723840613.405723.332826.1803 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- diff |.39240291.449801-2.4592763.244081 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ diff = mean(Loss) - mean(Gain)t =0.2707 Ho: diff = 0degrees of freedom =341 Ha: diff < 0Ha: diff != 0Ha: diff > 0 Pr(T < t) = 0.6066Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.7868Pr(T > t) = 0.3934 Since Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.7868 is greater than p-value 0.05 , the null hypothesis was accepted; gain-framed, compared to loss-framed, safety messages about mobile phone use while driving will be more effective in changing people’s intentions towards this behavior. • Hypothesis 2: High-issue involvement vs Low Issue involvement conditions The second hypothesis of this studyin relation to texting while drivingwas high issue involvement inducing, compared to low issue involvement inducing, safety messages will be more effective in changing people’s intentions about unsafe driving behaviour and was tested as shown below. Two-sample t test with equal variances ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Variable |ObsMeanStd. Err.Std. Dev.[95% Conf. Interval] ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- Low |1824.5686811.00878613.609272.5781886.559174 High |1614.9689441.04117213.210992.9127327.025156
Paraphrase This Document
Need a fresh take? Get an instant paraphrase of this document with our AI Paraphraser
PSYC105 REPORT 201811 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- combined |3434.75656.723840613.405723.332826.1803 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- diff |-.40026281.452366-3.2569862.456461 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ diff = mean(Low) - mean(High)t = -0.2756 Ho: diff = 0degrees of freedom =341 Ha: diff < 0Ha: diff != 0Ha: diff > 0 Pr(T < t) = 0.3915Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.7830Pr(T > t) = 0.6085 From the results, Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.0040 which is greater than the p-value 0.05 hence the null objective was accepted; high issue involvement inducing, compared to low issue involvement inducing, safety messages will be more effective in changing people’s intentions about unsafe driving behaviour. • Hypothesis 3: Gain-framed High-issue involvement vs. Loss-framed High-issue involvement The third hypothesis of this studyin relation to texting while drivingwas gain-framed safety messages about mobile phone use will be most effective when paired with a high issue involvement inducing message and was subsequently tested as shown below. Two-sample t test with equal variances ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ Variable |ObsMeanStd. Err.Std. Dev.[95% Conf. Interval] ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- Loss |814.8950621.25995211.339562.3876787.402445 Gain |805.043751.67053314.94171.7186388.368862 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- combined |1614.9689441.04117213.210992.9127327.025156 ---------+-------------------------------------------------------------------- diff |-.14868832.088889-4.2742363.976859 ------------------------------------------------------------------------------ diff = mean(Loss) - mean(Gain)t = -0.0712 Ho: diff = 0degrees of freedom =159 Ha: diff < 0Ha: diff != 0Ha: diff > 0 Pr(T < t) = 0.4717Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.9433Pr(T > t) = 0.5283 Since the Pr(|T| > |t|) = 0.9523 was more than the p-value, the null hypothesis was not rejected (failed to reject null hypothesis). Thus, gain-framed safety messages about mobile phone use will be most effective when paired with a high issue involvement inducing message.
PSYC105 REPORT 201812 The above findings about the three hypotheses were summarized in form of a box plot shown below. Low InvolvementHigh Involvement 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 Gain-framedLoss-framed Change in Intention to Text Graph representing the mean change in intention to text while driving between the four groups. From the graph it is evident that gain-framed, compared to loss-framed, safety messages about mobile phone use while driving will be more effective in changing people’s intentions towards this behavior. In addition, high issue involvement inducing, compared to low issue involvement inducing, safety messages will be more effective in changing people’s intentions about unsafe driving behaviour. Discussion The findings of the data analysis reveals that there is sufficient evidence to back the premise that some individual variances make some teenagers and young adult drivers more prone to calling/texting while driving than others. For example, those teenagers and young adult drivers who are certain that they are able to drive safely while calling or texting and those who have a sensation that their dear ones do not have an issue with them texting/calling while driving are more expected to do so (Nathanail & Adamos, 2013). Contrariwise, those teens and young adult drivers who perceive hidden texting and driving negatively, were less expected to engage in the behaviour (Chaurand, Bossart, & Delhomme, 2015). Based on these results, some submissions can be made concerning training targeting teens and young adult drivers. First, to help these group of people understand the real perils related with texting/calling
PSYC105 REPORT 201813 while driving, it may be indispensable for them to essentially experience the hazards for themselves (Millar & Millar, 2000). Thus, providing high or medium fidelity driving simulations that permit teenagers to text/call and drive in realistic, yet well-ordered, settings will be beneficial (Lewis, Watson, & Tay, 2007). In such cases, teens and young adult drivers will experience what it is essentially like to have a collision or nearly hit, hurt, or even kill another person since their eyes were off the road while calling or texting (Hoekstra & Wegman, 2011). Conclusion The systematic review discloses that young drivers are an at-risk group for side-tracked driving. This study recommend preventative policies based on detecting factors that sway drivers to engage in cell phone use while driving (CPWD measures found in other articles. Further studies are important to assess the efficiency of these suggested tactics. In addition, training of should emphasize to teens as well as the young adult drivers that concealed texting/calling is totally NOT acceptable. Apps for example Sprint Drive First which turns off phone call and text notifications while somebody is driving can assist in this effort as they do not depend on on somebody else sensing the actions first hand.
Secure Best Marks with AI Grader
Need help grading? Try our AI Grader for instant feedback on your assignments.
PSYC105 REPORT 201814 References Cazzulino, F., Burke, R. V., Muller, V., Arbogast. H. & Upperman, J. S. (2014). Cell Phones and Young Drivers: A systematic review regarding the association between psychological factors and prevention. Traffic Injury Prevention, 15, 234-242. Chaurand, N., Bossart, F. & Delhomme, P. (2015). A naturalistic study of the impact of message framing on highway speeding. Transport Research Part F, 35, 37-44. Darçın, M., & Alkan, M. (2015). Safety Risk of Mobile Phone Use While Driving In Sample of Taxi Drivers. Promet - Traffic& Transportation, 27(4), 21-49. doi:10.7307/ptt.v27i4.1597 Hallett, C., Lambert, A., & Regan, M. A. (2012). Text messaging amongst New Zealand drivers: Prevalence and risk perception. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 15(3), 261-271. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2011.12.002 Hoekstra, T. & Wegman, F. (2011). Improving the effectiveness of road safety campaigns: Current and new practices. IATSS Research, 34, 80-86. Miller, M. G. & Miller. K. U. (2000). Promoting Safe Driving Behaviours: The influence of message framing and issue involvement. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30 (4), 853-866. Waddell, L. P., & Wiener, K. K. (2014). What’s driving illegal mobile phone use? Psychosocial influences on drivers’ intentions to use hand-held mobile phones. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 22, 1-11. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2013.10.008 White, K. M., Hyde, M. K., Walsh, S. P., & Watson, B. (2010). Mobile phone use while driving: An investigation of the beliefs influencing drivers’ hands-free and hand-held mobile phone use. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 13(1), 9-20. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2009.09.004