Semantic Priming: Influence of Emotional Valence Words on Cognitive Processing
Verified
Added on  2023/01/11
|13
|3666
|27
AI Summary
This research examines the influence of emotional valence words on semantic priming and cognitive processing. It explores the priming effect at conscious and unconscious levels and how these processes relate to emotional valence words.
Contribute Materials
Your contribution can guide someone’s learning journey. Share your
documents today.
SEMANTIC PRIMING
Secure Best Marks with AI Grader
Need help grading? Try our AI Grader for instant feedback on your assignments.
TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION...........................................................................................................................3 METHOD........................................................................................................................................5 RESULTS........................................................................................................................................6 DISCUSSION..................................................................................................................................9 LIMITATIONS..............................................................................................................................10 CONCLUSION..............................................................................................................................10 REFERENCES..............................................................................................................................12
INTRODUCTION Cognitive psychologyhas been evolving regularly and the connection between stimulus, attention, consciousness and brain function still remains a highly studied but controversial area. The reading and processing of words can highly depend on whether a person has had a prior conscious perception of them or they perceive them for the first time. Conscious processing, however, is traditionally known as the extent to which important stimuli is given selective attentive space (Deutsch & Deutsch, 1963). Consciously, when a person is processing symbolic stimuli such as words, it requires an extensive cortical process. Word processing starts in the ventral visual pathway then processing to the lexical and lastly through semantic access (Gaillard, Del Cul, Naccache, Vinckier, Cohen &Dehaene, 2006). Visual consciousness and the rate words are detected depend on perceptual awareness, individual personality traits, lexical decision, and working memory. These processes are found to interact with each other during this conscious process. Processing of stimulimostly occurs when a person is conscious and aware as opposed to unconscious one. When being exposed to words influences a response to another word without conscious guidance or intent, priming occurs. However, if the words are semantically related such as table and chair, or unrelated like table and tiger,semantic primingoccurs. In visual word recognition, semantic priming is used as a powerful tool to research cognitive processes relating to memory, language, and attention. Some words, however, are not prime– target pairs but the human mind constitutes them as related. Examples of these kinds of words are book and worm or sky and blue. The priming effect has been proven to occur for word pairs that are associatively related, categorically related or simply look or sound similar (Okubo & Ogawa, 2013). Further when the unconsciousprocessing of the stimulithat isreceivedbecomes automatic, the process is termed assemantic activation. The semantic activation occurs at the deepest level of processing, as, Neely and Khan (2001), in their critical re-evaluation highlight semantic activation is indeed automatic. Words automatically activate their meanings once read slightly, and the semantic activation is unaffected by the intention for it to occur and by the amount and quality of the intentional resources allocated to it. Themasked semantic primingparadigm is often referred to as the ‘sandwich’ technique. This is due to the prime word being sandwiched between the pattern mask and the targeted stimulus (Forster, n.d.). Research in this area by Merikle, Smilek & Eastwood (2000) concludes 3
that stimulus information can be visually perceived even when there is no awareness of perceiving it. This is further supported by a study by Kinoshita & Norris (2012) who highlighted that this is related to the belief that an ‘unconscious’ prime will tap into automatic processes and can, therefore, be used to identify the obligatory representations and processes that support reading. Presentation of a masked prime word automatically causes alexical entryto be opened, if the same word is encountered a second time straight after the masked prime word, the lexical entry remains opened thus, having little effect on cognitive load (Bodner &Stalinski, 2008). However, depending on the multitude of information consciously thrown at our working memory, it can exceed a person’s capacity to process, thus overwhelming the brain and causing an effect on cognitive processing. This is explained asa cognitive load. This refers to the amount of mental activity imposed on our semantic memory at any one time. Though the semantic memorycanoverload,Neurosciencehasmappedacognitivepathwaythatdemonstrates unconscious processing, underlying emotional experience and maps out the road to unconscious processing. Even if stimuli have not been perceived consciously, the sub cortical pathway, otherwise known as the thalamus – amygdale connection may unconsciously perceive the word and its meaning (Mack & Rock, 1998). Consequently, unconsciously perceived stimuli can have an impact onaffective state, which in turn, is affected by a person’s personality traits. A person’s choices and preferences can have an automatic influence on a participant’s judgement on words whether positive or negative.A study conducted by Scott, Mogg & Bradley (2001), highlights that participants with a higher level of depression showed enhanced masked semantic priming of words surrounding depression as opposed to words related to happiness. Furthermore, four studies conducted by Rogers & Revelle (1998), discovered that personality factors influenced judgment when the choice was between positive and negative words. They concluded that mood factor highly influenced judgement and perception. Their research showed, however, that their cognitive load was not affected when choosing the word. Participants choose negative valance words a lot quicker than positive valance words. The present research will examine the influence of emotional valence words on a semantic priminglevel.Itwillreviewtheprimingeffectatconsciousandunconsciouscognitive processing and how these processes relate to emotional valence words. This research will look at three hypotheses that will support literature review: 4
Paraphrase This Document
Need a fresh take? Get an instant paraphrase of this document with our AI Paraphraser
1.It was hypothesized that the priming effect would be stronger for negative valance words as compared to positively valance words. 2.It was hypothesized that reaction times would be faster when prime words are related as compared to unrelated prime words. 3.It was hypothesized that participants who have higher anxiety tendencies will have a large priming effect across emotional valence words than a participant who has higher happiness tendencies. METHOD Participants Sixty-five students from a medium sized university in Melbourne participated in the experiment. All claimed to be native speakers of English. Materials Word stimuli.There were sixty target words, half of which were positive and half of which were negative. In addition to these target words there were thirty neutral words that were used as unrelated primes. This created sixty prime target pairs for each valence, thirty of which were related (repetition priming) and thirty were unrelated. The words in each group were balancedonpsycholinguisticcharacteristicsincludingwordfrequency,letterlength,and association strength using the website of Landauer and Dumais. Repetition priming was used such that each target word (in lower case) was primed by the same word in upper case (related prime-target pair), or was paired with a prime word (in upper case) of neutral emotional valence (unrelated prime-target pair). An example negative related repetition prime-target pair was TORTURE-torture, and the corresponding unrelated prime-target pair was JETTY-torture. Individual differences measures.The state items from the Spielberg State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (SSTAI; Spielberger et al., 1983) were used to provide a measure of anxiety, which is a negatively valenced emotion. Items from the Oxford Happiness Scale (OHS; Argyle et al., 1995) were used to provide a measure of happiness, which is a positive valenced emotion. Procedure Participants reached the experiment via a link on the Learning Management System. Participants were informed about the sequence of events in the task, and asked to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible. For each of the three main sections of the experiment, they completed 10 practice trials followed by 60 experimental trials. The first section of the 5
experiment asked participants to classify words presented on the screen as negative or positive emotional valence (Meaning Task). The second task repeated the meaning task, but this time in a dual task situation (Cognitive Load Task), where they were also asked to remember a pattern containing four x's and 4 o's in various configurations. After every five trials of the meaning task, they were asked to recall the current pattern, and then were asked to remember a new pattern. Following this, they were presented with a list of questions that they should answer based on their initial intuition without thinking too hard. The questions were from the two surveys, with the questions from the SSTAI (Spielberger et al., 1983) being presented first and the OHS second (Hills & Argyle, 2002). The experimental task and the two short surveys took about 15 minutes to complete. All of the participants performed the sequence of tasks in the same order without counterbalancing, beginning with the Meaning Task, followed by the Cognitive Load Task followed by the surveys. The instructions in the Meaning Task were designed to get participants to make a judgement about the words based on them being either of negative valence or positive valence. In the Cognitive Load Task, the participants were performing two tasks simultaneously. At the end of each block of trials, the participant were given performance feedback on latency to response and accuracy. In terms of the stimulus presentation, the main stimuli always appeared in the centre of the screen. The timing was as follows: (a) a forward letter mask appeared for 500 ms; (b) the prime was then presented for 48 ms; (c) a backward mask appeared for 96 ms; and (e) the target remained on the screen until the participant responded. In the Cognitive Load Task, the pattern to be remembered appeared on the screen for 2500 ms, then five trials of the Meaning Task occurred, and then the participant was asked to recall the pattern. The participant had 20 seconds to record their response, and were given feedback as to whether the entry was correct before being shown another pattern to remember. RESULTS Data were screened for response times that were less than 200 ms or greater than 1000 ms, and for incomplete data sets. The final data set contained data from 65 participants. To confirmthatparticipantsweresuccessfullycompletingthemeaningjudgementinboth conditions tasks, the percentage of correct responses was collated for all conditions, and presented in Table 1. 6
Table 1 Mean and SDs of the percentage of correct responses across conditions MeaningCognitive Load MeanSDMeanSD Percent correct94.10(5.19)94.97(4.31) As can be seen from this table, the mean percent correct for all tasks was well above 90% and there were no obvious differences across the two tasks. The mean percent correct for the pattern task was 82.11 (SD=15.63), confirming that participants were genuinely attempting the second task in the cognitive load condition. Reaction times as a function of emotional valence, task condition and prime relatedness are presented in Figure 1. Figure 1.Mean reaction times and error rates for identical and unrelated prime target pairs as a function of word valence and task condition. Note: REL=related, UNREL=unrelated, POS=positive, NEG=negative, MEAN=meaning task, LOAD=Cognitive load task. The error bars are +-1 SE. As can be seen from Figure 1, reaction times were faster in the related compared to the unrelated conditions.This difference in reaction times between related and unrelated prime- target pairs is referred to as a priming effect.When comparing related trials between the meaning and cognitive load conditions, reaction times appear generally faster for the load conditions.However, for the unrelated trials, the reaction times appear slightly slower in the 7
Secure Best Marks with AI Grader
Need help grading? Try our AI Grader for instant feedback on your assignments.
load task.In terms of priming effects, it appears the largest priming effects observed were for negative words in the cognitive load task, followed by positive words in the same task.The priming effects for the meaning tasks appear smaller than priming effects in the load condition, and appear relatively similar in size across valence. The same data from Figure 1 are presented to highlight priming effects for negatively and positively valenced words for the different task conditions in Figure 2. Figure 2. Mean priming effect as a function of word valence and task type. The error bars are +-1 SE. In order to determine whether the size of the priming effects differed between the meaning and cognitive load task, a 2 (relatedness) by 2 (task) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted. This revealed a significant main effect of relatedness (F(1, 64)=115.30, p<.001), suggesting a significant priming effect overall, and a significant interaction effect between task and relatedness (F(1, 64)=48.03,p<.001), with priming effects being greater in the cognitive load condition. A further two separate 2 (relatedness) by 2 (valence) ANOVAs were conducted to determine whether differences in priming effects were significant between words of a positive and negative valence for both the meaning and cognitive load conditions.For the meaning condition, the analysis revealed a significant main effect of relatedness (F(1, 64)=31.07,p<.001) and valence (F(1, 64)=19.27,p<.001), with RTs for negatively valenced words being faster than forpositivelyvalencedwords.However,therewasnosignificantinteractionbetween relatedness and valence (F(1, 64)=.44,p=.51), suggesting that priming effects for the meaning 8
conditions were relatively similar across valence.For the cognitive load condition, the analysis revealed significant main effects for both relatedness (F(1, 64)=94.34,p<.001) and valence (F(1, 64)=4.88,p=.03), again revealing RTs for negative trials were faster overall compared to positive trials. In contrast to the meaning task, a significant interaction between relatedness and valence was observed (F(1, 64)=22.06,p<.001), confirming the size of the priming effect for negatively valenced words was greater than that of positively valenced words within this cognitive load task condition. To examine any potential correlations between priming effects and individual difference variables, the anxiety (STAI) and happiness (OHS) scores were correlated with the size of the priming effect across emotional valence and task condition. While there was an expected negative correlation between anxiety and happiness, (r = -.82, p < .001), none of the correlations of specific interest to the research hypotheses were significant. DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to examine the semantic priming effect in conscious and unconsciousprocessingandhowtheseprocessesrelatetoemotionalvalencewords. Furthermore, it reviews how individual differences such as anxiety and happiness can affect a persons' cognitive processing. The first hypothesis was supported as the results show that a significant priming effect sizes occurred for both positive and negative valance words; however, there were no results for neutral words. The largest priming effect that occurred was for the negative words in the cognitive load task. Selection of these negative words was automatic showing that the priming effect was not reduced under the cognitive load. With the events of co-occurrence within the larger text of the task and word frequencies, the negative valence word was able to be computed a lot quicker. Positive valance words usually have a higher number of associations than negative valance words and therefore, they may be more prone to elicit semantic integration (Kuhlmann, Hofmann, Briesemeister & Jacobs, 2016). The second hypothesis was also supported as expected stating that participants reaction times would be faster when prime words in the target pair are related than unrelated prime words. When faced with related prime target pairs, participants reacted a lot faster as compared to unrelated prime target pairs. When comparing tasks, reaction times were generally faster for the cognitive load and when the words have a negative valence. Meyer and Schvaneveldt (1971), 9
state that our long-term memory is organised semantically, in other words, there is a structure within our long-term memory in which the location of two associated words are closer than those of two associated words. If the word is familiar to the participant, the starting location to cognitively process this word is in the familiar sector of the lexical decision making. This is generally faster; a major shift between locations in the brain is required to access potential information about unrelated words, whereas such a shift is not required for related words. Third hypothesis was the unsupported hypothesis where contrary to expectations, a negative correlation was found between happiness and anxiety. LIMITATIONS However this does not show enough correlations of specific interest between priming effect and individual methodological problem that might have interfered with the ability to find a correlation between individual differences such as anxiety and happiness and persons' cognitive. By extending the survey and including more emotions, such as depression and apprehensive for negative valance and self-worth, and health traits for the positive valance, it would have helped in analysing wider aspects. The involvement of negative and positive emotions such as anxiety and happiness have briefly been explored, but it is equally important to explore how mental health traits such as anxiety and happiness correlate to prime words both negative and positive and how these words provoke an emotional response for participants.Additionally, further research is needed to investigate the impact of emotion- evoking words on behaviour. CONCLUSION In conclusion, this study has highlighted some important key findings surrounding priming effect on conscious and unconscious levels, however further research is needed to establish whether or not emotional valence words affected participant’s mental health traits of feeling anxious or happiness. It was highlighted that a significant priming effect did occur when participants viewed emotional valence words. For negative valance words particularly, the size of the priming effect was greater than positively valance words. The reaction time for negative prime words was significantly faster than positive prime words, showing that negative words increased unconscious processing speed particularly when the targeted words were related. Overall,regardlessofresultsonlysupportingtwooutofthethreehypotheses, comprehensively results demonstrate how powerful the connection between stimulus, attention, 10
Paraphrase This Document
Need a fresh take? Get an instant paraphrase of this document with our AI Paraphraser
consciousness and brain function is and how someone’s perception of words can still influence conscious behaviour through reaction time and valance. 11
REFERENCES Bodner, G.E., &Stalinski, S.M.. (2008). Masked repetition priming and proportion effects under cognitive load. CanadianJournalofExperimentalPsychology,6(2), 127-131. Deutsch,J.A.,&Deutsch,D.(1963).Attention:Sometheoreticalconsiderations. PsychologicalReview,70, 80-90. Heyman, T., Van Rensbergen, B., Storms, G., Hutchison, K. A., & De Deyne, S. (2015). The influenceofworkingmemoryloadonsemanticpriming. JournalofExperimentalPsychology:Learning,Memory,andCognition, 41(3), 911-920. Hills, P., & Argyle, M. (2002). The Oxford happiness questionnaire: A compact scale for the measurementofpsychologicalwell-being. PersonalityandIndividualDifferences,33,1073-1082. Merikle,P.M.,Smilek,D.,&Eastwood,J.D.(2001).Perceptionwithoutawareness: perspectives from cognitive psychology. Cognition,79, 115-134. Meyer,D.E.,L.Schvaneveldt,R.W.(1971). Facilitationinrecognizingpairsofwords:Evidenceofadependencebetweenretrievalop erations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1971, 90, 227-234. Neely, J.H.& Kahan, T.A.. (2001). Is semantic activation automatic? A critical re-evaluation. In: Roedinger,H.L.,Naime,J.S.,&Suprenant.,A.M.(Eds).Thenatureof remembering.EssaysinhonourofRobertG.Crowder. pp 69-93.Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. 12