logo

The Social Psychology of Discrimination: Theory, Measurement and Consequences

   

Added on  2023-06-10

29 Pages10948 Words361 Views
84
Chapter 5
The Social Psychology of Discrimination:
Theory, Measurement and Consequences
Ananthi Al Ramiah, Miles Hewstone,
John F. Dovidio and Louis A. Penner
ocial psychologists engage with the prevalence and problems
of discrimination by studying the processes that underlie it.
Understanding when discrimination is likely to occur suggests
ways that we can overcome it. In this chapter, we begin by dis
cussing the ways in which social psychologists talk about dis
crimination and discuss its prevalence. Second, we outline some
theories underlying the phenomenon. Third, we consider the
ways in which social psychological studies have measured dis
crimination, discussing findings from laboratory and field stud
ies with explicit and implicit measures. Fourth, we consider the
systemic consequences of discrimination and their implications
for intergroup relations, social mobility and personal wellbeing.
Finally, we provide a summary and some conclusions.
Defining Discrimination
Social psychologists are careful to disentangle discrimination
from its close cousins of prejudice and stereotypes. Prejudice re
fers to an unjustifiable negative attitude toward a group and its
individual members. Stereotypes are beliefs about the personal
S
The Social Psychology of Discrimination: Theory, Measurement and Consequences_1
The Social Psychology of Discrimination
85
attributes of a group of people, and can be overgeneralised, inac
curate, and resistant to change in the presence of new informa
tion. Discrimination refers to unjustifiable negative behaviour to
wards a group or its members, where behaviour is adjudged to
include both actions towards, and judgements/decisions about,
group members. Correll et al. (2010, p. 46) provide a very useful
definition of discrimination as ‘behaviour directed towards cate
gory members that is consequential for their outcomes and that is
directed towards them not because of any particular deserving
ness or reciprocity, but simply because they happen to be mem
bers of that category’. The notion of ‘deservingness’ is central to
the expression and experience of discrimination. It is not an ob
jectively defined criterion but one that has its roots in historical
and presentday inequalities and societal norms. Perpetrators
may see their behaviours as justified by the deservingness of the
targets, while the targets themselves may disagree. Thus the be
haviours, which some judge to be discriminatory, will not be seen
as such by others.
The expression of discrimination can broadly be classified
into two types: overt or direct, and subtle, unconscious or auto
matic. Manifestations include verbal and nonverbal hostility
(Darley and Fazio, 1980; Word et al., 1974), avoidance of contact
(Cuddy et al., 2007; Pettigrew and Tropp, 2006), aggressive ap
proach behaviours (Cuddy, et al., 2007) and the denial of oppor
tunities and access or equal treatment (Bobo, 2001; Sidanius and
Pratto, 1999).
Across a range of domains, cultures and historical periods,
there are and have been systemic disparities between members
of dominant and nondominant groups (Sidaniuis and Pratto,
1999). For example, ethnic minorities consistently experience
worse health outcomes (Barnett and Halverson, 2001;
Underwood et al., 2004), worse school performance (Cohen et
al., 2006), and harsher treatment in the justice system
(Steffensmeier and Demuth, 2000). In both business and aca
The Social Psychology of Discrimination: Theory, Measurement and Consequences_2
Making Equality Count
86
demic domains, women are paid less and hold positions of lower
status than men, controlling for occupation and qualifications
(Goldman et al., 2006). In terms of the labour market, sociologi
cal research shows that ethnic minority applicants tend to suffer
from a phenomenon known as the ethnic penalty. Ethnic penal
ties are defined as the net disadvantages experienced by ethnic
minorities after controlling for their educational qualifications,
age and experience in the labour market (Heath and McMahon,
1997). While the ethnic penalty cannot be equated with dis
crimination, discrimination is likely to be a major factor respon
sible for its existence. This discrimination ranges from unequal
treatment that minority group members receive during the ap
plication process, and over the course of their education and so
cialisation, which can have grave consequences for the existence
of ‘bridging’ social networks, ‘spatial mismatch’ between labour
availability and opportunity, and differences in aspirations and
preferences (Heath and McMahon, 1997).
Theories of Discrimination
Several theories have shaped our understanding of intergroup
relations, prejudice and discrimination, and we focus on four
here: the social identity perspective, the ‘behaviours from inter
group affect and stereotypes’ map, aversive racism theory and
system justification theory.
As individuals living in a social context, we traverse the
continuum between our personal and collective selves. Differ
ent social contexts lead to the salience of particular group
memberships (Turner et al., 1987). The first theoretical frame
work that we outline, the social identity perspective (Tajfel and
Turner, 1979) holds that group members are motivated to pro
tect their selfesteem and achieve a positive and distinct social
identity. This drive for a positive social identity can result in
discrimination, which is expressed as either direct harm to the
The Social Psychology of Discrimination: Theory, Measurement and Consequences_3
The Social Psychology of Discrimination
87
outgroup, or more commonly and spontaneously, as giving
preferential treatment to the ingroup, a phenomenon known as
ingroup bias.
Going further, and illustrating the general tendency that
humans have to discriminate, the minimal group paradigm stud
ies (Tajfel and Turner, 1986) reveal how mere categorisation as a
group member can lead to ingroup bias, the favouring of in
group members over outgroup members in evaluations and allo
cation of resources (Turner, 1978). In the minimal group para
digm studies, participants are classified as belonging to arbitrary
groups (e.g. people who tend to overestimate or underestimate
the number of dots presented to them) and evaluate members of
the ingroup and outgroup, and take part in a reward allocation
task (Tajfel and Turner, 1986) between the two groups. Results
across hundreds of studies show that participants rate ingroup
members more positively, exhibit preference for ingroup mem
bers in allocation of resources, and want to maintain maximal
difference in allocation between ingroup and outgroup mem
bers, thereby giving outgroup members less than an equality
norm would require. Given the fact that group membership in
this paradigm does not involve a deeplyheld attachment and
operates within the wider context of equality norms, this ten
dency to discriminate is an important finding, and indicative of
the spontaneous nature of prejudice and discrimination in inter
group contexts (Al Ramiah et al., in press). Whereas social cate
gorisation is sufficient to create discriminatory treatment, often
motivated by ingroup favouritism, direct competition between
groups exacerbates this bias, typically generating responses di
rectly to disadvantage the outgroup, as well (Sherif et al., 1961).
Whereas social identity theory examines basic, general proc
esses leading to intergroup discrimination, the BIAS map (Be
haviours from Intergroup Affect and Stereotypes; see Cuddy, et
al., 2007) offers insights into the specific ways that we discrimi
nate against members of particular types of groups. The BIAS
The Social Psychology of Discrimination: Theory, Measurement and Consequences_4
Making Equality Count
88
map (Figure 1) is an extension of the Stereotype Content Model
(Fiske et al., 2002), and proposes that the relative status and
competitiveness of groups determine the stereotype content of
warmth and competence attributed to the outgroup.
Figure 1: The BIAS Map: Behaviours from Intergroup Affect and
Stereotypes (Cuddy et al., 2007)
These stereotypes predict affect towards the outgroup, and af
fect predicts action tendencies. Group stereotypes contain a
mixture of competence and warmth attributes, and this combi
nation of content gives rise to particular emotions and action
tendencies. The warmth dimension of stereotypes, which carries
greater weight in social encounters (Cuddy et al., 2007; Van
Lange and Kuhlman, 1994; Vonk, 1999; Wojciszke et al., 1998),
predicts active behavioural tendencies while the secondary di
mension of competence predicts more passive tendencies. Nega
tive active and passive behaviours can be construed by targets as
constituting discrimination, and can have significant impact on
The Social Psychology of Discrimination: Theory, Measurement and Consequences_5
The Social Psychology of Discrimination
89
the quality of their lives. Examples of negative passive behav
iours are ignoring another’s presence, not making eye contact
with them, excluding members of certain groups from getting
opportunities, and so on, while examples of negative active be
haviours include supporting institutional racism or voting for
antiimmigration political parties. These examples show that
discriminatory behaviours can range from the subtle to the
overt, and the particular views that we have about each out
group determines the manifestation of discrimination.
The third theory that we consider, aversive racism (Dovidio
and Gaertner, 2004) complements social identity theory (which
suggests the pervasiveness of intergroup discrimination) and the
BIAS map (which helps identify the form in which discrimina
tion will be manifested) by further identifying when discrimina
tion will be manifested or inhibited. The aversive racism frame
work essentially evolved to understand the psychological con
flict that afflicts many White Americans with regard to their ra
cial attitudes. Changing social norms increasingly prohibit
prejudice and discrimination towards minority and other stig
matised groups (Crandall et al., 2002), and work in the United
States has shown that appearing racist has become aversive to
many White Americans (Dovidio and Gaertner, 2004; Gaertner
and Dovidio, 1986; Katz and Hass, 1988; McConahay, 1986) in
terms not only of their public image but also of their private self
concept. However, a multitude of individual and societal factors
continue to reinforce stereotypes and negative evaluative biases
(which are rooted, in part, in biases identified by social identity
theory), which result in continued expression and experience of
discrimination. Equality norms give rise to considerable psycho
logical conflict in which people regard prejudice as unjust and
offensive, but remain unable to fully suppress their own biases.
Thus ethnic and racial attitudes have become more complex
than they were in the past.
The Social Psychology of Discrimination: Theory, Measurement and Consequences_6

End of preview

Want to access all the pages? Upload your documents or become a member.

Related Documents
Managing diversity and equality in the workplace
|5
|800
|132