Analyzing the Effects of Mobile Phone Use on Safe Driving
VerifiedAdded on 2023/06/03
|15
|2215
|245
Report
AI Summary
This report analyzes the behavior of using mobile phones while driving, focusing on the impact on driver safety and the effectiveness of safety messages. The research, based on a survey of 343 participants, investigates the effects of gain-framed and loss-framed safety messages, as well as the role of high and low issue involvement in changing drivers' intentions. The study found that texting and calling while driving is a common behavior, and all three hypotheses were supported. The study also revealed that safety messages are effective in changing drivers' intentions and that high issue involvement can improve message effectiveness. The report highlights the complexity of the collected data as a limitation and suggests further research on other risky driving behaviors to enhance safety. The research includes statistical analysis of participant demographics, driving experience, and mobile phone use habits, and it presents the results through tables and graphs, including intention scores and differences between pre and post-intention scores regarding phone calls and texting while driving.
Contribute Materials
Your contribution can guide someone’s learning journey. Share your
documents today.

1
Running head: SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The Behavior of using Mobile Phones when Driving
Student’s Name
Institutional Affiliation
Date
Running head: SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The Behavior of using Mobile Phones when Driving
Student’s Name
Institutional Affiliation
Date
Secure Best Marks with AI Grader
Need help grading? Try our AI Grader for instant feedback on your assignments.

2
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
Introduction
Our safety is of great importance and needs to be maintained at all times (Geller, 2016).
We have some critical conditions where our safety is normally at great risk and in such
conditions, all the necessary measures should be put in place to make sure our safety is
guaranteed as much as possible. One of such critical conditions which tend to risk our safety
greatly is when driving. While driving, our safety is normally at great risk since a slight mess on
the driver’s side can lead to some fatal consequences (Kalra and Paddock, 2016, pp.182-193).
The behavior of the drivers using their mobile phones when driving has been found to be one of
the major behaviors which highly compromise their safety and the safety of the passengers in
their vehicles. This behavior is a great problem in our lives as some of the drivers who use their
phones when driving lose concentration and may end up over-speeding their vehicles or engage
in other risky behaviors which can cause serious accidents (Hoekstra and Wegman, 2011, pp.80-
86). Therefore, this problem needs to be addressed comprehensively to address the many
problems which result from the behavior of using mobile phones when driving. Much research
has been done to see how this behavior of using mobile phones when driving can be eliminated
but the research has not been very successful (Cazzulino, Burke, Muller, Arbogast, and
Upperman, 2014, pp.234-242). More research continues to be done to help in eliminating this
behavior, and perhaps one day in the future, the behavior will be eliminated completely where
we’ll have no driver using his/her mobile phone when driving (Zhou and Curry, 2015). This
paper will analyze the behavior of using mobile phones when driving which will help us to know
how this behavior can be addressed, and this is very important as it will help to enhance our
safety.
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
Introduction
Our safety is of great importance and needs to be maintained at all times (Geller, 2016).
We have some critical conditions where our safety is normally at great risk and in such
conditions, all the necessary measures should be put in place to make sure our safety is
guaranteed as much as possible. One of such critical conditions which tend to risk our safety
greatly is when driving. While driving, our safety is normally at great risk since a slight mess on
the driver’s side can lead to some fatal consequences (Kalra and Paddock, 2016, pp.182-193).
The behavior of the drivers using their mobile phones when driving has been found to be one of
the major behaviors which highly compromise their safety and the safety of the passengers in
their vehicles. This behavior is a great problem in our lives as some of the drivers who use their
phones when driving lose concentration and may end up over-speeding their vehicles or engage
in other risky behaviors which can cause serious accidents (Hoekstra and Wegman, 2011, pp.80-
86). Therefore, this problem needs to be addressed comprehensively to address the many
problems which result from the behavior of using mobile phones when driving. Much research
has been done to see how this behavior of using mobile phones when driving can be eliminated
but the research has not been very successful (Cazzulino, Burke, Muller, Arbogast, and
Upperman, 2014, pp.234-242). More research continues to be done to help in eliminating this
behavior, and perhaps one day in the future, the behavior will be eliminated completely where
we’ll have no driver using his/her mobile phone when driving (Zhou and Curry, 2015). This
paper will analyze the behavior of using mobile phones when driving which will help us to know
how this behavior can be addressed, and this is very important as it will help to enhance our
safety.

3
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The behavior of using mobile phones when driving forms the basis of our main research
questions which can be stated as:
1. Are the available safety messages about using mobile phones when driving effective, and
do they change the drivers’ intentions about this behavior?
2. Can the effectiveness of these safety messages be improved by inducing high issue
involvement in individuals?
From the research questions stated above, we can form the following hypotheses:
The gain-framed safety messages are more effective than the loss-framed safety messages
about using mobile phones when driving and work better in changing the drivers’ intentions
towards this behavior.
High issue involvement inducing is more effective than low issue involvement inducing,
and helps to improve the effectiveness of these safety messages in changing the drivers’
intentions towards this behavior than low issue involvement inducing.
Gain-framed safety messages about this behavior of using mobile phones when driving
will be more effective when combined with high issue involvement inducing message.
Method
Participants
A total of 892 participants participated in the survey conducted during the research, but
not all of them were serious in the survey as some did not provide all the required information.
All the incomplete data which was provided by some participants and the data given by
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The behavior of using mobile phones when driving forms the basis of our main research
questions which can be stated as:
1. Are the available safety messages about using mobile phones when driving effective, and
do they change the drivers’ intentions about this behavior?
2. Can the effectiveness of these safety messages be improved by inducing high issue
involvement in individuals?
From the research questions stated above, we can form the following hypotheses:
The gain-framed safety messages are more effective than the loss-framed safety messages
about using mobile phones when driving and work better in changing the drivers’ intentions
towards this behavior.
High issue involvement inducing is more effective than low issue involvement inducing,
and helps to improve the effectiveness of these safety messages in changing the drivers’
intentions towards this behavior than low issue involvement inducing.
Gain-framed safety messages about this behavior of using mobile phones when driving
will be more effective when combined with high issue involvement inducing message.
Method
Participants
A total of 892 participants participated in the survey conducted during the research, but
not all of them were serious in the survey as some did not provide all the required information.
All the incomplete data which was provided by some participants and the data given by

4
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
participants above 26 years old was removed since it was not necessary for the research. The
remaining data was from 343 participants, and this data was adequate for our research.
Measures
The main measure which was used in this research was the intention measure. The
intention measure was applied in the research to help in measuring the drivers’ intentions about
the use of mobile phones when tired, and this intention measure helped to achieve the desired
results. This intention measure is a modified version of the intention measure which was used by
Hatfield, Murphy, Kasparian, and Job (2005) to measure the young drivers’ intentions about
driving when they are tired.
Procedures
The participants of the research (drivers) were informed about the study and its intentions orally,
and those willing to participate went ahead to provide the required information. ‘Driving while
using a mobile phone’ was taken as the intention variable of the research. To analyze the
collected data, the scores from different questions were averaged to obtain the intention score.
The texting and talking intention scores were calculated using the questions about the
participants’ behaviors. The mobile phone use intention difference score was calculated by
subtracting the second mobile phone (text/talk) use intention score from the first mobile phone
(text/call) use intention score or simply subtracting the post-intention score from the pre-
intention score. The higher the difference score (positive), the lower is the likelihood of doing
the behavior of texting or talking when driving.
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
participants above 26 years old was removed since it was not necessary for the research. The
remaining data was from 343 participants, and this data was adequate for our research.
Measures
The main measure which was used in this research was the intention measure. The
intention measure was applied in the research to help in measuring the drivers’ intentions about
the use of mobile phones when tired, and this intention measure helped to achieve the desired
results. This intention measure is a modified version of the intention measure which was used by
Hatfield, Murphy, Kasparian, and Job (2005) to measure the young drivers’ intentions about
driving when they are tired.
Procedures
The participants of the research (drivers) were informed about the study and its intentions orally,
and those willing to participate went ahead to provide the required information. ‘Driving while
using a mobile phone’ was taken as the intention variable of the research. To analyze the
collected data, the scores from different questions were averaged to obtain the intention score.
The texting and talking intention scores were calculated using the questions about the
participants’ behaviors. The mobile phone use intention difference score was calculated by
subtracting the second mobile phone (text/talk) use intention score from the first mobile phone
(text/call) use intention score or simply subtracting the post-intention score from the pre-
intention score. The higher the difference score (positive), the lower is the likelihood of doing
the behavior of texting or talking when driving.
Secure Best Marks with AI Grader
Need help grading? Try our AI Grader for instant feedback on your assignments.

5
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The results
The first set of the results which shows the demographic information of the participants is shown
in the tables below:
From the tables, we can see that the data of 343 participants were analyzed. Of the 343
participants, 86 were male, 255 were female, and 2 refused to disclose their gender. The ages of
the participants ranged between 17 years (minimum) to 26 years (maximum). The average age of
the participants was 19.207 years, and the standard deviation was 1.682646.
The second set of results which shows the driving experience of the participants and their
behaviors when driving is shown in the tables below:
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The results
The first set of the results which shows the demographic information of the participants is shown
in the tables below:
From the tables, we can see that the data of 343 participants were analyzed. Of the 343
participants, 86 were male, 255 were female, and 2 refused to disclose their gender. The ages of
the participants ranged between 17 years (minimum) to 26 years (maximum). The average age of
the participants was 19.207 years, and the standard deviation was 1.682646.
The second set of results which shows the driving experience of the participants and their
behaviors when driving is shown in the tables below:

6
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
From the tables above, we can see that 342 participants disclosed information about their
licensing where the participants who had been licensed for the longest period had been licensed
for 10 years and we had some who had not been licensed. The results also revealed that 6 drivers
were not licensed, 74 had learners type of licenses, 213 had Ps (public service) type of licenses,
43 had full/heavy vehicle type of licenses, and 7 had motorcycle licenses.
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
From the tables above, we can see that 342 participants disclosed information about their
licensing where the participants who had been licensed for the longest period had been licensed
for 10 years and we had some who had not been licensed. The results also revealed that 6 drivers
were not licensed, 74 had learners type of licenses, 213 had Ps (public service) type of licenses,
43 had full/heavy vehicle type of licenses, and 7 had motorcycle licenses.

7
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The first table above which shows the drivers’ behaviors of using their mobile phones
when driving show that 47 drivers use their mobile phones three times or more on weekly basis
when driving, 93 drivers use their mobile phones once or twice in a week when driving, 61
drivers use their phones ones in a month when driving, and 107 drivers don’t use their phones
when driving. The other table which was based on the views of these drivers about the behavior
of using mobile phones when driving of the other drivers shows that these drivers thought that
194 other drivers use their mobile phones when driving three or more times in a week, 118 other
drivers use their phones when driving once or twice in a week, 21 other drivers use their mobile
phones when driving once or twice a month, 6 other drivers use their mobile phones when
driving less than once in a month, and 4 other drivers never use their mobile phones when
driving.
In testing the hypotheses using the measures of the drivers’ intentions to make/take phone
calls (talk) when driving, the following results were obtained:
The difference score is a bit high (positive) which means the chance of the drivers to talk when
driving is very low.
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The first table above which shows the drivers’ behaviors of using their mobile phones
when driving show that 47 drivers use their mobile phones three times or more on weekly basis
when driving, 93 drivers use their mobile phones once or twice in a week when driving, 61
drivers use their phones ones in a month when driving, and 107 drivers don’t use their phones
when driving. The other table which was based on the views of these drivers about the behavior
of using mobile phones when driving of the other drivers shows that these drivers thought that
194 other drivers use their mobile phones when driving three or more times in a week, 118 other
drivers use their phones when driving once or twice in a week, 21 other drivers use their mobile
phones when driving once or twice a month, 6 other drivers use their mobile phones when
driving less than once in a month, and 4 other drivers never use their mobile phones when
driving.
In testing the hypotheses using the measures of the drivers’ intentions to make/take phone
calls (talk) when driving, the following results were obtained:
The difference score is a bit high (positive) which means the chance of the drivers to talk when
driving is very low.
Paraphrase This Document
Need a fresh take? Get an instant paraphrase of this document with our AI Paraphraser

8
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The difference score is negative which means the chance of the drivers to talk when driving is
very high.
The difference score is positive but very low which means the chance of the driver talking when
driving is slightly high.
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The difference score is negative which means the chance of the drivers to talk when driving is
very high.
The difference score is positive but very low which means the chance of the driver talking when
driving is slightly high.

9
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The information conveyed by the results of the test hypotheses above can be graphically
represented by the graphs below:
In testing the hypotheses using the measures of the drivers’ intentions to text when driving,
the following results were obtained:
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The information conveyed by the results of the test hypotheses above can be graphically
represented by the graphs below:
In testing the hypotheses using the measures of the drivers’ intentions to text when driving,
the following results were obtained:

10
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The difference score is positive and slightly high which means the chance of the driver texting
when driving is slightly low.
The difference score is negative which means the chance of the driver texting when driving is
slightly high.
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The difference score is positive and slightly high which means the chance of the driver texting
when driving is slightly low.
The difference score is negative which means the chance of the driver texting when driving is
slightly high.
Secure Best Marks with AI Grader
Need help grading? Try our AI Grader for instant feedback on your assignments.

11
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The difference score is negative which means the chance of the driver texting when driving is
high.
The information conveyed by the hypotheses results above can be graphically represented
by the graphs below:
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The difference score is negative which means the chance of the driver texting when driving is
high.
The information conveyed by the hypotheses results above can be graphically represented
by the graphs below:

12
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
Discussion
From our research, we’ve found that texting and calling when driving is a common
behavior with many drivers. Testing the hypotheses also proved all our hypotheses right.
Therefore, all our three hypotheses have been proved to be facts. The research also helped to
answer our research questions where we have learned that the available safety messages about
using mobile phones when driving are effective, and do change the drivers’ intentions about the
behaviors of using their mobile phones when driving. The research has also revealed that the
effectiveness of the safety messages can be greatly improved by inducing high issue involvement
messages in individuals (Millar and Murray, 2000, pp.853-866).
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
Discussion
From our research, we’ve found that texting and calling when driving is a common
behavior with many drivers. Testing the hypotheses also proved all our hypotheses right.
Therefore, all our three hypotheses have been proved to be facts. The research also helped to
answer our research questions where we have learned that the available safety messages about
using mobile phones when driving are effective, and do change the drivers’ intentions about the
behaviors of using their mobile phones when driving. The research has also revealed that the
effectiveness of the safety messages can be greatly improved by inducing high issue involvement
messages in individuals (Millar and Murray, 2000, pp.853-866).

13
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The major strength of the study is that it has helped us to understand the gain-framed
safety messages and the low-framed safety messages and how these messages affect the
behaviors of the drivers when driving (Chaurand, Bossart, and Delhomme, 2015, pp.37-44). This
knowledge can be applied to come up with some good measures which can help to address this
major challenge of drivers talking or texting when driving which pauses a serious threat to our
safety. The major limitation of the study was on the complexity of the collected data which made
the analysis of the data quite challenging. There remains much to be researched on the behaviors
of the drivers when driving and doing detailed research on this to improve the behaviors of
drivers when driving will help to minimize many accidents which are associated with many
deaths (Carter, Bingham, Zakrajsek, Shope, and Sayer, 2014, pp.S32-S41). The use of mobile
phones by drivers when driving is not the only risky behavior portrayed by drivers, but there are
many other behaviors which need more research to be addressed and solved to improve our
safety.
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
The major strength of the study is that it has helped us to understand the gain-framed
safety messages and the low-framed safety messages and how these messages affect the
behaviors of the drivers when driving (Chaurand, Bossart, and Delhomme, 2015, pp.37-44). This
knowledge can be applied to come up with some good measures which can help to address this
major challenge of drivers talking or texting when driving which pauses a serious threat to our
safety. The major limitation of the study was on the complexity of the collected data which made
the analysis of the data quite challenging. There remains much to be researched on the behaviors
of the drivers when driving and doing detailed research on this to improve the behaviors of
drivers when driving will help to minimize many accidents which are associated with many
deaths (Carter, Bingham, Zakrajsek, Shope, and Sayer, 2014, pp.S32-S41). The use of mobile
phones by drivers when driving is not the only risky behavior portrayed by drivers, but there are
many other behaviors which need more research to be addressed and solved to improve our
safety.
Paraphrase This Document
Need a fresh take? Get an instant paraphrase of this document with our AI Paraphraser

14
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
References
Carter, P. M., Bingham, C. R., Zakrajsek, J. S., Shope, J. T., & Sayer, T. B. (2014). Social norms
and risk perception: Predictors of distracted driving behavior among novice adolescent
drivers. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54(5), S32-S41.
Chaurand, N., Bossart, F., and Delhomme, P. (2015). A naturalistic study of the impact of
message framing on highway speeding. Transportation Research Part F, 35, 37-44.
Francesca Cazzulino , Rita V. Burke , Valerie Muller , Helen Arbogast & Jeffrey S. Upperman
(2014) Cell Phones and Young Drivers: A Systematic Review Regarding the Association
Between Psychological Factors and Prevention, Traffic Injury Prevention, 15:3, 234-242,
DOI: 10.1080/15389588.2013.822075
Geller, E. S. (2016). The psychology of safety handbook. Florida: CRC Press.
Hatfield, J., Murphy, S., Kasparian, N., & Job, R. (2005). Risk perceptions, attitudes, and
behaviors regarding driver fatigue in NSW youth: The Development of an Evidence-
Based Driver Fatigue Educational Intervention Strategy. Report to the Motor Accidents
Authority of NSW.
Kalra, N., & Paddock, S. M. (2016). Driving to safety: How many miles of driving would it take
to demonstrate autonomous vehicle reliability? Transportation Research Part A: Policy
and Practice, 94, 182-193.
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
References
Carter, P. M., Bingham, C. R., Zakrajsek, J. S., Shope, J. T., & Sayer, T. B. (2014). Social norms
and risk perception: Predictors of distracted driving behavior among novice adolescent
drivers. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54(5), S32-S41.
Chaurand, N., Bossart, F., and Delhomme, P. (2015). A naturalistic study of the impact of
message framing on highway speeding. Transportation Research Part F, 35, 37-44.
Francesca Cazzulino , Rita V. Burke , Valerie Muller , Helen Arbogast & Jeffrey S. Upperman
(2014) Cell Phones and Young Drivers: A Systematic Review Regarding the Association
Between Psychological Factors and Prevention, Traffic Injury Prevention, 15:3, 234-242,
DOI: 10.1080/15389588.2013.822075
Geller, E. S. (2016). The psychology of safety handbook. Florida: CRC Press.
Hatfield, J., Murphy, S., Kasparian, N., & Job, R. (2005). Risk perceptions, attitudes, and
behaviors regarding driver fatigue in NSW youth: The Development of an Evidence-
Based Driver Fatigue Educational Intervention Strategy. Report to the Motor Accidents
Authority of NSW.
Kalra, N., & Paddock, S. M. (2016). Driving to safety: How many miles of driving would it take
to demonstrate autonomous vehicle reliability? Transportation Research Part A: Policy
and Practice, 94, 182-193.

15
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
Millar, U.K., and Murray. G.M. (2000). Promoting Safe Driving Behaviors: The Influence of
Message Framing and Issue Involvement. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(4),
853-866.
BIBLIOGRAPHY Wegman, F. and Hoekstra, T. (2011). Improving the effectiveness of road safety
campaigns: Current and new practices. IATSS Research, 34, 80-86.
Zhou, X., & Curry, W. (2015). U.S. Patent No. 8,971,927. Washington, DC: U.S. Patent and
Trademark Office.
SAFE-DRIVING BEHAVIORS
Millar, U.K., and Murray. G.M. (2000). Promoting Safe Driving Behaviors: The Influence of
Message Framing and Issue Involvement. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(4),
853-866.
BIBLIOGRAPHY Wegman, F. and Hoekstra, T. (2011). Improving the effectiveness of road safety
campaigns: Current and new practices. IATSS Research, 34, 80-86.
Zhou, X., & Curry, W. (2015). U.S. Patent No. 8,971,927. Washington, DC: U.S. Patent and
Trademark Office.
1 out of 15
Related Documents

Your All-in-One AI-Powered Toolkit for Academic Success.
+13062052269
info@desklib.com
Available 24*7 on WhatsApp / Email
Unlock your academic potential
© 2024 | Zucol Services PVT LTD | All rights reserved.